It is well
known that stress can impair memory. Everyone has had some experience of this
kind. As a student suffering test anxiety, grades are likely to suffer. In
high-stakes social or business interactions, the stress may well cause memory
to fail us, as when Presidential candidate Rick Perry forgot the name of the
agency he wanted to abolish if elected, or when we forget a friend’s name in
the process of making a social introduction. How does stress do this? Is there
anything we can do about it?
First, we need to know what stressful events do to the body and brain. Brain
freezes, like Rick Perry's, probably occur because thinking can get so preoccupied
with the stress-inducing stimuli that other thoughts cannot emerge. But other
kinds of stress-induced memory impaired come from the well-known “fight or
flight” response in which stress activates the release of adrenalin into the
blood stream. Adrenalin has many bodily effects that support fight or flight,
such as raising heart rate and blood pressure, and increasing arousal perhaps
to the point of anxiety and fear. The increased attentiveness may have a
fleeting beneficial effect on memory, as has been demonstrated in laboratory
experiments. But the other effects of adrenalin on anxiety and distress are
likely to impair memory.
The other thing that happens during stress is the activation of the
anterior pituitary gland’s release of ACTH, which in turn activates another
part of the adrenal gland to dump cortisol into the blood stream. In the short
term, cortisol can have many beneficial effects for combatting stress, such as
mobilizing white blood cells and enhancing the immune system. But cortisol
binds to cells in the brain’s hippocampus, the area that converts new
experiences into memory. This binding actually disrupts the memory-forming
process. Ultimately, if stress continues, the synaptic regions deteriorate,
making the impairment permanent.
The effects of both adrenalin and cortisol were revealed in an
interesting study of mild social stress. Here, the focus was on a theory of how
stress effects on memory might be thwarted by a learning technique called
forced retrieval. Prior research with students, had shown, that the usual study
technique of re-reading notes or text is not nearly as effective as requiring
the learner to actively retrieve the information, as one might do, with flash
cards, for example. Just a few months ago, I posted a blog on this forced
retrieval phenomenon as a key element in “strategic studying.”
This new research was aimed at testing the possibility that forced
retrieval might protect learners from the memory deficits caused by stress. In
the study on the first day, 120 subjects studied a list of 30 nouns or images
of nouns one at a time. Then, one half of the group restudied the items while
the other half practiced retrieval by recalling as many items as they could
(but without feedback telling them if they got it right). One the next day,
half of each group were stressed by being required to solve hard math problems
and by giving speeches in front of two judges and three peers. Then they were
tested. Twenty minutes later they took a second test on items that had not been
tested on the first test. The results revealed that retrieval practice yielded
better results.
On the first test, we see that the stressed learners who just studied the
items the day before had fewer of the items remembered on the first test given
immediately after the stress. But there was no such effect on the stressed
leaners who used retrieval practice during the initial learning. This
protective effect of retrieval practice was evident on the second test 25
minutes later. In fact, the retrieval practice effect was better than on the
first test, even though different items were tested. You may have noticed that the
stressed study group on the second test did worse than they did on the first
test. This is attributed to a mild effect of adrenalin, which as mentioned
above can have some benefit on memory. Adrenalin’s action is immediate and is
apparently swamped on the second test by the delayed release of cortisol, which
shows up by the second test. Students might note that the magnitude of
difference may appear small, but in percentage terms could equal to more than two
letter grades (compare the two stressed groups on the delayed test).
To explain why forced retrieval works, the authors speculate that it
provides better initial encoding. That is, the new information is registered
more strongly if you make yourself try to retrieve it. This is consistent with the
everyday experience that most of us have had wherein information that strongly
grabs our attention is more likely to be remembered. Forced retrieval is a way
to make ourselves pay better attention to what we are trying to lean.
Readers wanting to learn more about
improving memory are urged to check “Memory Medic’s” books, Memory Power 101 and Better
Grades, Less Effort.
Sources:
Klemm, W. R.
(2016). Strategic studying. October 9, http://thankyoubrain.blogspot.com/2016/10/strategic-studying.html
Smith, Amy
M. et al. (2016). Retrieval practice protects memory against acute stress.
Science. 354 (6315), 1046-1047.