As explained in my memory-improvement
book, "Memory Power 101," the most powerful way to remember something
is to construct a mental-image representation. All the memory books I have read
make the same point. The professional memorizers, "memory athletes"
who can memory the sequence of four shuffled decks of cards in five minutes,
all use some form of mental imaging that converts each card into a mental-picture
representation.
Now a recent experiment documents the power of mental images
in a study involving seven experiments that compared memory accuracy with
whether or not a drawing was made. College-student volunteers were asked to
memorize a list of words, each of which was chosen to be easily drawn. Words
were presented one at a time on a video monitor and students were randomly
prompted to write the name of the object or make a drawing of it. Each word
presentation was timed and a warning buzzer indicated it was time to stop and
get ready for the next word display. At the end of the list, a two-minute
filler task was presented wherein each student classified 60 sound tones,
selected at random, in terms of whether the frequency was low, medium, or high.
Then a surprise test was given wherein students were asked to verbally recall in
one minute as many words as they could, in any order, whether written or drawn.
In the first two experiments students remembered about twice
as many when a drawing representation had been made than when just the word had
been written. Three other experiments demonstrated that drawing was more
effective because the encoding was deeper. For example, one experiment was conducted
like the first two, but included a third condition in which the subjects were
to write a list of the physical characteristics of the word (for example, for
apple, one might say red, round, tasty, chewy, etc.). This presumably provides
a deeper level of encoding than just writing or
drawing the word. Results
revealed that drawing was still more effective than either writing a list of
attributes or writing the word.
Another highly important experiment was conducted that
compared drawing and writing with just making a mental image without drawing
it. Again, drawing produced the best results, although more words were
remembered when mentally imaged than when written.
A follow-on experiment substituted an actual picture of the
word instead of requiring the student to actively imagine an image. Here again,
best results occurred with drawing, with seeing pictures being more effective
than writing the word.
In a sixth experiment, drawing was still superior to writing
even if the list of words was made longer or if the encoding time was reduced.
In the last experiment, drawing was still beneficial in a way that could not be
explained solely by the fact that drawings are more distinctive than writing a
word.
The benefits of drawing were seen within and across
individuals and across different conditions. The researchers concluded that
drawing improves memory by encouraging a seamless integration of semantic,
visual, and motor aspects of a memory trace. That makes sense to me.
The processes involved here that account for better memory
are 1) elaborating the item to be remembered, 2) making a mental image of it or
an alias for it, 3) the motor act of drawing the image, and 4) the reinforcing
feedback of thinking about the drawing.
The implications of studies like this have enormous
practical application for everyday needs to remember. The principle is that
whenever you have something you need to remember, make a mental-image
representation of it and then draw it. For example, if you have to remember
somebody named "Mike" make a mental image of the person speaking into
a microphone (mike). Then roughly draw Mike's main facial features alongside a
microphone. There are all sorts of formal schemes for making mental images,
even for numbers, as explained in my book. This present study indicates that
the making of a mental image is powerfully reinforced when you try to draw it.
To some extent, this memory principle is used in elementary
school, where drawing is a huge part of the curriculum. As students get older,
teachers abandon drawing and usually so do the students. Perhaps educators need
to revisit the idea that drawing has educational value at all grade levels.
Source:
Kluger, Jeffrey, (2016) Here's the
memory trick that science says works. Time, April 22. http://time.com/4304589/memory-picture-draw/
Wammes, Jeffrey D. et al. (2016. The
drawing effect: Evidence for reliable and robust memory benefits in free
recall. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 69 (9), 1752-1776. DOI:10.1080/17470218.2015.1094494
Thanks for sharing such fantastic stuff. I like very much your thoughts. keep continue.
ReplyDeletehttps://play.google.com/store/apps/details?id=ias.upsc.ibps.rrb.exam&rdid=ias.upsc.ibps.rrb.exam
I have read some of your blogs and I found all of them very much expressive and easy to understand. Some people have it very clear in their mind that how to express the ideas and views. I think you are one of them. Thank you for this.
ReplyDeleteMelanotan Nasal Spray
I agree that the best way to remember something is to draw them or picture them in your mind.
ReplyDeleteThank you for sharing this to us. I'm sure it will help me a lot.
ReplyDeleteThank you all for your encouragement.
ReplyDeleteGood one... Having a good memory could lead you to make things without any fail. You will get many benefits like you could perform well whatever you on.
ReplyDeleteMemory Improvement Techniquest
Concentration Exercises For Students
Physics And Maths Tutor
One On One Maths Tutoring