Teenagers are notorious for poor decision-making. Of course
that is inevitable, given that their brains are still developing, and they have
had relatively little life experience to show them how to predict what works
and what doesn’t. Unfortunately, what doesn’t work may have more emotional
appeal, and most of us at any age are more susceptible to our emotions than
cold, hard logic.
Seniors also are prone to poor
decision-making if senility has set it. Unscrupulous people take advantage of
such seniors because a brain that is deteriorating has a hard time making wise
decisions.
In between teenage and senility
is when the brain is at its peak for good decision making. Wisdom comes with
age, up to a point. Some Eastern cultures venerate their old people as
generally being especially wise. After all, it you live long enough, and are still
mentally healthy, you ought to make good decisions because you have a lifetime
of experience to teach you what future choices are likely to work and which are
not.
Much of that knowledge comes from
learning from one’s mistakes. On the other hand, some people, regardless of
age, can’t seem to learn from their mistakes. Most of the time the problem is
not stupidity but a flawed habitual process by which one is motivated to make
wise decisions and evaluate options. Best of all is learning from somebody else’s mistakes, so you don’t have to
make them yourself.
Learning from your mistakes can
be negative, if you fret about it. Learning what you can do to avoid repeating
a mistake is one thing, but dwelling on it erodes one’s confidence and sense of
self worth. I can never forget the good advice I read from, of all people, T.
Boone Pickens. He was quoted in an interview as saying that he was able to
re-make his fortune on multiple occasions because he didn’t dwell on losing the
fortunes. He credited that attitude to his college basketball coach who told
the team after each defeat, “Learn from your mistakes, but don’t dwell on them.
Learn from what you did right and do more of that.”
It would help if we knew how the
brain made decisions, so we could train it to operate better. “Decision
neuroscience” is an emerging field of study aimed at how learning how brains
make decisions and how to optimize the process. Neuroscientists seemed to have
honed in on two theories, both of which deal with how the brain handles the
processing of alternate options to arrive at a decision.
One theory is that each option is
processed in its own competing pool of neurons. As processing evolves, the
activity in each pool builds up and down as each pool competes for dominance.
At some point, activity builds up in one of the pools to reach a threshold, in
winner-take-all fashion, to allow the activity in that pool to dominate and
issue the appropriate decision commands to the parts of the brain needed for
execution. As one possible example, two or more pools of neurons separately
receive input that reflects the representation of different options. Each pool
sends an output to another set of neurons that feed back either excitatory or
inhibitory influences, thus providing a way for competition among pools to
select the pool that eventually dominates because it has built up more impulse
activity than the others.
The other theory is based on guided gating wherein input to
pools of decision-making neurons is gated to regulate how much excitatory
influence can accumulate in each given pool. [i]
The specific routing paths involve inhibitory neurons that shut down certain
routes, thus preferentially routing input to a preferred accumulating circuit.
The route is biased by estimated salience of each option, current emotional
state, memories of past learning, and the expected reward value for the outcome
of each option.
These decision-making possibilities
involve what is called “integrate and fire.” That is, input to all relevant
pools of neurons accumulates and leads to various levels of firing in each
pool. The pool firing the most is most likely to dominate the output, that is,
the decision.
However circuits make decisions,
there is considerable evidence that nerve impulse representations for each
given choice option simultaneously code for expected outcome and reward value.
These value estimates update on the fly.[ii]
Networks containing these representations compete to arrive at a decision.
Any choice among alternative
options is affected by how much information for each option the brain has to
work on. When the brain is consciously trying to make a decision, this often
means how much relevant information the brain can hold in working memory.
Working memory is notoriously low-capacity, so the key becomes remembering the
sub-sets of information that are the most relevant to each option. Humans think
with what is in their working memory. Experiments have shown that older people
are more likely to hold the most useful information in working memory, and
therefore they can think more effectively. The National Institute of Aging
began funding decision-making research in 2010 at Stanford University’s Center
on Longevity. Results of their research are showing that older people often
make better decisions than younger people.
As one example, older people are
more likely to make rational cost-benefit analyses. Older people are more
likely to recognize when they have made a bad investment and walk away rather
than throwing more good money after bad.
A key factor seems to be that
older people are more selective about what they remember. For example, one
study from the Stanford Center compared the ability of young and old people to
remember a list of words. Not surprisingly, younger people remembered more
words, but when words were assigned a number value, with some words being more
valuable than others, older people were better at remembering high-value words
and ignoring low-value words. It seems that older people selectively remember
what is important, which should make it easier to make better decisions.
Decision-making skills are
important of learning achievement in school. Students need to know how to focus
in general, and focus on what is most relevant in particular. They are not
learning that skill, and their multi-tasking culture is teaching them many bad
habits.
Those of us who care deeply about
educational development of youngsters need to push our schools to address the
thinking and learning skills of students. "Teaching to the test"
detracts from time spent in teaching what matters most. Today's culture of
multi-tasking is making matters worse. Children don't learn how to attend
selectively and intensely to the most relevant information, because they are
distracted by superficial attention to everything. Despite their daily use of
Apple computers and smart phones, only one college student out of 85 could draw
the Apple logo correctly.[iii]
Memory training is generally
absent from teacher training programs. Despite my locally well-publicized
experience in memory training, no one in the College of Education at my
university has ever asked me to share my knowledge with their faculty or with pre-service
teachers. The paradox is that teachers are trained to help students remember
curricular answers for high-stakes tests. What could be more important than
learning how to decide what to remember and how to remember it? And we wonder
why student performance is so poor?
"Memory Medic" is author of Memory Power 101 (Skyhorse) and Better Grades, Less Effort (Benecton).
[i]Purcell,
B. A.; Heitz, R. P.; Cohen, J. Y.; Schall, et al. (2010). Neurally constrained
modeling of perceptual decision making. Psychological Review, 117(4), 1113-1143.
[ii]
McCoy, A. N., and Platt, M. L. (2005). Expectations and outcomes:
decision-making in the primate brain. J. Comp. Physiol A 191, 201-211.
[iii]
Blake, Adam B., Nazarian, Meenely, and Castel, Alan D. (2015). The Apple of the
mind's eye: Everyday attention, metamemory, and reconstructive memory for the
Apple logo. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2015; 1 DOI:
10.1080/17470218.2014.1002798
No comments:
Post a Comment
Please contribute your ideas. This blog is all about making learning easier for everyone.