Get enough sleep to consolidate
your memories.
Did you know that your brain works while you sleep?
Yes, both during dreaming and non-dreaming, your brain is consolidating
memories of events in the immediately preceding day.
Most people think that the purpose of sleep is to rest the brain. But there is clear evidence that the brain is still busily at work during sleep, even when the brain is not dreaming. Decades ago, researchers demonstrated that many neurons fired just as much during sleep as during wakefulness. Some neurons were even more active during sleep.
One advantage that
sleep provides for memory consolidation is that the brain doesn’t have all the
distractions that occur during daytime wakefulness. Multiple-conflicting
stimuli and tasks are very disruptive to memory consolidation.
The advantages offered
by having fewer disruptive influences during sleep have also been confirmed in
a study conducted in the brain imaging lab of Thomas Pollmacher in Munich,
Germany. An auditory text stimulus was presented to sleep-deprived subjects
prior to and after the onset of sleep, and imaging was performed to compare
wakeful responses to sound stimuli with those during various stages of
non-dreaming sleep. Brain activity during sleep was suppressed in auditory
pathways and visual cortex, including other brain regions that are
interconnected with the visual cortex. Suppression suggests that sleep shields
the brain from the arousing effects of external stimulation that might disturb
sleep. Blocking out such interference effects should facilitate memory consolidation.
This study also prompted researchers to conclude that consolidation of memory
occurs over many hours, at least in sleep-deprived subjects. That is to be
expected, inasmuch as consolidation of memory depends on protein synthesis and
physical changes in synapses.
Students often cut back
on sleep to finish ever-mounting piles of homework and study. Combat soldiers
are trained to function under sleep-deprived conditions. But these strategies are
likely counter-productive. At my university, our Corps of Cadets used to have a
tradition of rousing freshmen in the middle of the night and preventing them
from sleeping. The idea was to make them tough. More likely, it just made them
unable to do well in school, as I have seen many of them flunk out. Another area where this problem has surfaced
is with sleep-deprived medical residents.
Sleep loss degrades
many brain functions. In one study, sleep loss degraded visual vigilance and
memory for words, and time-of-day fluctuations were found in choice reaction
time, logical reasoning, and word memory. Exercise also seemed to have an
effect in that brain function of non-exercising subjects degraded sooner than they
did for exercising subjects. So, sleep-deprived couch potatoes beware!
Researchers have found
that people who stay up all night after learning and practicing a new task show
little improvement in their performance. No amount of sleep on following nights
can make up for the toll taken by the initial all-nighter.
Robert Stickgold and
colleagues at Harvard Medical School report that people who learned a
particular task did not improve their performance when tested later the same
day but did improve after a night of sleep. To see whether the night of sleep
actually caused the improvement, Stickgold trained 24 subjects in the same
visual discrimination task, which consisted of identifying the orientation of
three diagonal bars flashed for a sixtieth of a second on the lower left
quadrant of a computer screen full of horizontal stripes. Half of the subjects
went to sleep that night, while the other half were kept awake until the second
night of the study. Both groups were allowed to sleep on the second and third
nights. On the fourth day, both groups were tested on the visual discrimination
task. Those who slept the first night identified the correct orientation of the
diagonal bars much more rapidly than they had the first day. The other group
showed no improvement, despite the two nights of catch-up sleep.
Another compelling
study for the role of sleep on memory consolidation was published by Sean
Drummond and his colleagues at San Diego State University and the University of
California, San Diego. They combined
memory performance with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) to study sleep
deprivation effects on verbal learning of young, healthy adults. After a
sleepless night, free recall fell by about half, and the brain imaging analysis
showed reduced blood oxygen activity in the temporal area. However, the areas
of the prefrontal cortex that had been activated during remembering after
normal sleep worked even more after sleep deprivation. What's more, the
bilateral parietal lobes and two additional areas in the prefrontal cortex,
usually not activated after normal sleep, became active.
What about a small
degree of sleep loss? A University of Pennsylvania study showed even a little
sleep loss can devastate memory. People were assigned to sleep regimens of
four, six, or eight hours of sleep each night for two weeks and tested
periodically during the daytime for mental performance. Subjects who got four
or even six hours of sleep performed as poorly on brain function tests as they
did when kept from sleeping at all for three consecutive days. So,
short-changing your sleep each night by an hour or so builds up a sleep debt
that affects attention and working memory. In the study, performance decline was
cumulative. An interesting aside from the study was that none of the 48 people
in the study realized that their mental performance had deteriorated from the
mild sleep loss. As a college professor, I wonder about the performance loss
going on in students who short-change their sleep for months at a time.
There are also studies
revealing lack of sleep BEFORE learning interferes with memory. Formally, this
is called "proactive interference," because it occurs in advance. The
cause may relate to what was just explained: a sleepy brain doesn’t think
effectively.
In another study, 28
healthy young adults were divided into two groups. On the first day, one group
was kept awake for 35 straight hours. Participants in the other group spent a
normal sleep night at home. At 6 PM of the next day, all subjects watched a
slide show of 150 slides of landscapes, objects, and people who weren't
celebrities. All subjects then were sent home to have a normal night's sleep. The
next evening all subjects took a pop quiz on the slides, which were randomly
mixed with 75 new slides. The test was for subjects to recognize whether they
had seen each slide before.
Those subjects who had
been sleep deprived on the first night scored the lowest, even though they
later had a night to catch up on lost sleep. The upshot of it all is that lack
of sleep is bad for remembering, whether the sleep loss occurs before or after
learning events. For those who wonder why humans need to sleep, one obvious
benefit is to enhance learning.
Need to learn something
quickly? Take a nap. Daytime naps are said to rejuvenate energy and lower
stress. Now there is evidence naps speed up consolidation of memories.
Matthew Walker reports
experiments showing nap enhancement of memory. In his study, 39 young adults
were divided into two groups. At noon, all the participants took part in a
memory exercise that required them to remember faces and link them with names.
Then the subjects took part in another memory exercise at 6 p.m., after 20 subjects
had napped for 100 minutes during the break. Those who remained awake performed
about 10 percent worse on the tests than those who napped. Students take note:
10% is often the difference between an A and a B.
Sleep Sources
Drumond, Sean, Brown, Gregory, G., Gillin, J. Chrisstian,
Stricker, John. L. (2000). Altered brain response to verbal learning following
sleep deprivation. Nature 403(6770):655-7. DOI: 10.1038/35001068
Stickgold, R., James, L, and Hobson, J. (2000) Nature
Neuroscience. 3 (12), 1237-1238. DOI:10.1038/81756
Van Dongen, H.P.A., Rogers, N.L. & Dinges, D.F. Sleep
debt: Theoretical and empirical issues. Sleep Biol. Rhythms 1, 5–13 (2003).
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1446-9235.2003.00006.x
Walker, Matthew (2010). American Association for the
Advancement of Science annual meeting presentation, San Diego, Feb. 21, 2010.
This concludes our lessons in this series on Learning How to Learn.
I believe and hope that you all will become more
effective life-long learners.
No comments:
Post a Comment
Please contribute your ideas. This blog is all about making learning easier for everyone.