As far back as Plato and Aristotle, people believed that our
memories had to be physical somethings that were stored somewhere in the brain.
But only in modern times have we learned much about what this something is.
First, the something was given a name: memory engram. Then, as knowledge
accumulated about what happens in neurons and their synapses as they become
active in learning and remembering, it became clear that learning events that
could be remembered were causing chemical and physical changes in the junctions
(synapses) between neurons that participate in the learning experience.
Participating neurons grow new dendritic branches (called spines) and the
synapses on those spines enlarge and their neurotransmitter systems become
enhanced. These changes constitute the engram. Post-learning reactivation of
the synapses holding such an engram can produce recall of the original learning
that created the engram.
In the early days of neuroscience, scientists believed that
learning experiences assigned or recruited certain parts of the brain to hold
the memory. An experimenter, Karl Lashley, taught certain tasks to lab animals,
and then under anesthesia, destroyed different parts of the neocortex in the
hopes of finding where the memory was stored. He couldn’t find any particular
storage location. What he did find was that the more extensive he made the
cortical lesions, the more likely he could erase the memory. In other words,
memory of a given experience seemed to be deconstructed and parceled out into
different regions.
Then came quantitative EEG studies by E. Roy John, in which
he tracked the location of brain electrical evoked responses in different parts
of the cortex during learning experiences. He saw that a given learning
experience would produce electrical responses in several parts of the cortex,
again suggesting a deconstruction and distribution of memory engrams. This led
him to famously proclaim, “Memory is not a thing in a place, but a process in a
population.” Well, we know that this is
a bit of over-statement. There is such a thing as a memory engram that is
stored in specific places. Nonetheless, there is a distribution process for
creating the engram in multiple locations and for orchestrating them into
simultaneous and coordinated activity during recall of the memory.
Modern genetic engineering and neuron staining technology provide
powerful new tools to examine neurons that participate in joining the neural circuits
involved engrams. There are now ways to image and manipulate engrams at the
level of neuronal ensembles. Several
lines of evidence show that engram neurons can be seen histologically and
evaluated under various experimental approaches. For example, histological stains
revealing neurons that are activated by a learning experience show that they are
also active during memory retrieval of that experience. Second,
loss-of-function studies show that impairing engram neuron function after an
experience impairs subsequent memory retrieval. Third, studies show that memory
retrieval can be triggered by optogenic stimulation of engram neurons in the
absence of any natural sensory retrieval cues.
The basic approach used by
investigators in the lab of Susumu Tonegawa was to teach mice to avoid walking
into a chamber in which they would receive a mild electric shock. Neurons that
are activated by this fear conditioning fluoresce in immunohistological stains
of brain slices in mice that are sacrificed at various times after learning reveal
a memory engram that resides in selected neurons in the amgydala (which processes fear information),
in the hippocampus (which converts short-term memory to longer-term memory),
and in multiple regions of neocortex (which holds long-term memory in the form
of enhanced synaptic capability). Some of these cells still
fluoresce when examined many days later, indicating that they have become part
of an ensemble of engram neurons that hold a relatively lasting representation
of the original learned experience.
Other
mice were genetically engineered so that engram cells would fluoresce and be activated
when exposed to light delivered via micro-fiber optic cables surgically
implanted in various regions of neocortex. Such light stimulation of engram
cells confirmed their engram status, because light stimulation alone triggered
the previously learned behavior (freezing in place, rather than entering the
shock chamber). A key finding was that engram neurons in the prefrontal cortex
were “silent” soon after learning — they could initiate freezing behavior when
artificially activated by light delivered via surgically implanted fiber optic
filaments, but they did not fire during natural memory recall. In other words,
the memory engram was formed right away in all three places (amygdala,
hippocampus, and neocortex), but the engram cells in the neocortex had to
mature over time to become fully functional.
Over the
next two weeks, the engram neurons in the neocortex gradually matured, as
reflected by changes in their anatomy and physiological activity. By the end of
that same period, the hippocampal engram cells became silent and were no longer
used for natural recall. At this point, the mice could recall the event
naturally, without activation of neocortical cells by fiber-optic light. However,
traces of the memory remained in the hippocampus, because reactivating those
hippocampal neurons with light prompted the animals to freeze.
The past
prevailing view was that learning experiences are temporarily held in circuits
in the hippocampus and then later exported out to other parts of brain for
final storage. Both in the past and now, all the evidence indicates that the
hippocampus is crucial for forming lasting memories of experiences that do not
involve motor learning, but the mechanisms had been uncertain.
Neuroscientists
did know that long-term memories were stored outside of the hippocampus,
because people with hippocampal damage can lose the ability to form new
long-term memories, but they are still able to recall old memories.
Now, the new
research suggests that memory engrams are not transported from hippocampus to
neocortex but are present in both places at the outset of learning. The memory
engram in the neocortex just requires maturation for the memory to become more
permanent. Moreover, the hippocampus cannot, and need not, hold long-lasting
engrams.
Though
this is a new way to think about the mechanisms of how temporary memories
consolidate into longer-lasting ones, the conventional idea of consolidation
remains confirmed. That is, the memory engram must mature over time in the form
of biochemical and anatomical changes in the engram cells. Obviously, such
maturation process would be disrupted if those same engram cells are recruited
to serve other learning purposes before they have finished their maturation as
a specific memory engram. This also helps to explain why subsequent rehearsals
help make memories last longer, because each rehearsal re-engages engram
neurons into the same kind of activity they performed during learning, thus
strengthening the relevant synapses.
Once
memories were formed in the fear-conditioned mice, the engram cells in the
amygdala remained unchanged throughout the course of the experiment. Those
cells, which are necessary to evoke the emotions linked with specific memories,
like fear of entering the shock chamber in this case, communicate with engram
cells in both the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex.
We don’t
know what happens to memory-specific engram cells in the hippocampus. Maybe as
they gradually lose their engram status, they become available for processing
new kinds of learning experience. Perhaps some traces of engram remain in
hippocampus and are accessible for reactivation if highly relevant inputs are
received, as could be the case with strong memory cues. Perhaps the important
point is that these new techniques for labeling engram cells open the door for new
ways to study of memory retrieval, the long-neglected aspect of memory
mechanisms.
Another
potentially relevant finding of this kind of research is that memory engrams
may become damaged but may still exist in a form that cannot be retrieved by
natural means. The fact that such “silent” engrams can be retrieved with direct
optogenetic stimulation indicates that failures to recall do not necessarily
indicate that the memory is lost. The problem may lie in an inadequacy of the natural
memory cues used to triggger memory retrieval.
The door
is also now open for experiments that might advance our understanding of the
maturation of engram neurons in the neocortex. What is known so far is that
maturation requires initial communication with engram cells in the hippocampus.
Disrupting hippocampal connections between hippocampus and frontal cortex
prevents the maturation of neocortical engram cells.
Sources
Takashi
Kitamura, Takashi, et al., (2017). Engrams and circuits crucial for systems
consolidation of a memory,” Science, 356(6333), 73-78; DOI:
10.1126/science.aam6808
thank you for studies to learn easily. Animation notes
ReplyDeletethank you for studies to learn easily. Animation notes
ReplyDelete