“Life, liberty, and the
pursuit of happiness:” some people might argue that the U.S. Constitution
endorses hedonism, and indeed many politicians want to ignore or get rid of the
Constitution, but not necessarily because of hedonism. We should not be
dismissive about encouraging people to pursue happiness. Happiness can be good
for your brain. Depression is surely bad for your brain.
Positive mood states
promote more effective thinking and problem solving. A recent scholarly report[1]
reviews the literature demonstrating that positive mood broadens the scope of
attentiveness, enhances semantic associations over a wide range, improves task
shifting, and improves problem-solving capability. The review also documents
the changes in brain activation patterns induced by positive mood in subjects
while solving problems. Especially important is the dopamine signaling in the
prefrontal cortex.
Published studies reveal
that a variety of techniques are used to momentarily manipulate mood. These
have included making subjects temporarily happy or sad by asking subjects to
recall emotionally corresponding past experiences or to view film clips or hear
words that trigger happy or sad feelings,
The effect of happiness
on broadened attentiveness arises because the brain has better cognitive
flexibility and executive control, which in turn makes it easier to be more
flexible and creative. Happy problem solvers are better able to select and act
upon useful solutions that otherwise never consciously surface. Happiness
reduces perseverative tendencies for errant problem-solving strategies. The
broadened attentiveness, for example, allows people to attend to more stimuli,
both in external visual space and in internal semantic space, which in turn
enables more holistic processing. For example, in one cited study,
experimenters manipulated subjects’ momentary mood and then measured
performance on a task involving matching of visual objects based on their
global versus local shapes. Happy moods yielded better global matching.
Other experiments report
broader word association performance when subjects are manipulated to be
happier. For example, subjects in a neutral mood would typically regard the
word “pen” as a writing tool and would associate it with words like pencil or
paper. But positive mood subjects would think also of pen as an enclosure and
associate it with words like barn or pigs. This effect has been demonstrated
with practical effect in physicians, who, when in a happy mood, thought of more
disease possibilities in making a differential diagnosis.
The review authors
reported their own experiment on beneficial happy mood effects on insightfulness,
using a task in which subjects were given three words and asked to think of a
fourth word that could be combined into a compound word or phrase. For example,
an insightful response to “tooth, potato, and heart” might be “sweet tooth,
sweet potato, and sweetheart.” Generating such insight typically requires one
to suppress dominant “knee jerk” responses such as associating tooth with pain
and recognizing that pain does not fit potato while at the same time becoming
capable of switching to non-dominant alternatives.
Other cited experiments
showed that happy mood improved performance on “Duncker’s candle task.” Here, subjects are given a box of tacks, a candle,
and a book of matches, and are asked to attach a candle to the wall in a way
that will burn without dripping wax on the floor. Subjects in a happy mood were
more able to realize that the box could be a platform for the candle when the
box is tacked to the wall.
Such effects of happy
moods seem to arise from increased neural activity in the prefrontal cortex and
cingulate cortex, areas that numerous prior studies have demonstrated as
crucial parts of the brain’s executive control network. Similar effects have
been observed in EEG studies. Other research suggests that the happiness effect
is mediated by increased release of dopamine in the cortex that serves to
up-regulate executive control.
The review authors
described a meta-analysis of 49 positive-psychology manipulation studies
showing that momentary happiness is readily manipulated by such strategies as
deliberate optimistic thinking, increased attention to and memory of happy
experiences, practicing mindfulness and acceptance, and increasing
socialization. The effect occurs in most normal people and even in people with
depression, anxiety, and schizophrenia. Biofeedback training, where subjects
monitor their own fMRI scans or EEGs, might be an even more effective way for
people to train themselves to be happier.
The main point is that
people can be as happy as they choose to be.
For more
on how positive mood influences memory ability,
see my
new book, Memory Power 101 (http://skyhorsepublishing.com).
[1]
Subramaniam, K. and Vinogradov, S. (2013). Improving the neural mechanisms of
cognition through the pursuit of happiness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 7
August. Doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00452
No comments:
Post a Comment
Please contribute your ideas. This blog is all about making learning easier for everyone.