I just attended a “memory schema” symposium at the annual
meeting of the Society of Neuroscience. The “schema” idea is that memory of
prior learning provides a framework or context for new learning. That is, new
information is evaluated for relevance to preexisting schema, which may
influence how readily new information transfers into memory.
The notion of schema stems originally from Harry Harlow’s
ideas back in the 1940s. Harlow showed that when a monkey learns a new kind of
problem, he solves it by slow plodding trial and error. However, if he has
experience with a large number of problems of a similar type or class, the
trial and error is replaced by a process in which the individual problems are
eventually solved insightfully. For example, if you learn how to do task A, B,
and C, when presented with a new task D, you might say to yourself, “I don’t
know how to do this task D, but it is like task B, and I do know how to do
that!” Thus, you have a leg up on learning how to do task D. The idea underlies
how people become experts in a given field: their accumulated learning of
various tasks provides them with a repertoire of what Harlow called “learning
sets” that makes it easier to learn new things.
Few of the speakers or audience discussants seemed to be
aware of this literature, and their ideas weren’t really all that new, except
that the focus is now shifting to memory instead of insight. The basic idea of
memory schemas is that associations among learning objects profoundly affect
how easily and well a person can remember. Certainly, memory is promoted when
learning objects are congruent, that is, have meaningful relationships.
Sometimes, however, you can easily remember incongruent items because they are
so different. These ideas are important to education, and in the panel
discussion at the end of the symposium, speakers were asked to address this
matter. But nobody did. And in school systems, few educators do either.
Master teachers have always known intuitively to structure
meaningful relationships among learning objects. In principle, this is done by
creating associations of word pairs, concepts, spatial locations, and assorted
rules and principles. All these things make it easier for students to learn.
The problem is that we educators don’t devote enough thought about practical
ways to create structured relationships that will promote memory formation and
recall. I don’t follow much of the educational research literature, but I suspect
that very little of it focuses on the best way to organize the presentation of
learning materials. For instance, has anybody conducted an experiment that
tests how well students learn the central concepts in the U.S. constitution and
its amendments, depending on how the concepts are presented? Or what’s the best
way to structure learning objects in the teaching of cell organelles and their
functions? Typically, in the latter case for example, a biology teacher
considers each organelle in turn and spews out information on what it does.
That may not be the most memorizable way to present the information. Maybe it
would be better to begin with the biological needs of the cell, how those needs
relate to each other, and then how various organelles fulfill those needs. In
fact, I took that kind of approach in the on-line biology curriculum I wrote, but no experiment has
compared the ease of learning this way versus the traditional approach. I do
know from my own experience with trying to learn a little Spanish that the ease
of memorizing verb conjugations was greatly affected by how I laid out the
words in a table.
To return the schema symposium, the experiments reported
made it clear that structured relationships of learning objects improve all
aspects of learning: encoding, memory consolidation, and recall. The time has
come to develop teaching strategies that exploit the brain’s preferred mode of
operation.
One example is the development of a PowerPoint method I
developed to create a one-flash card of learning objects that consists of
mnemonic icons systematically placed in specified spatial locations. The images
represent concepts to memorize, and the spatial locations create spatial
relationships that promote memory of the learning objects. For details, see my e-book for students.
In more general terms, the primary task of teachers and
students is to develop strategies to enrich the formation of memory schemas.
This means finding ways to increase the number and congruence of associations
among facts and concepts being taught. The research shows that major benefits
can be expected.
Source:
Harlow H F. 1949. The formation of learning sets. Psychol.
Rev. 56:51-65.
Klemm, W. R. 2012. Better Grades, Less Effort. (e-book in
all formats at Smashwords.com)
Van Kesteren, M. T. R., and Henson, R. N. A. 2012. The
re-emergence of schemas in memory research: from encoding to reconsolidation.
Society of Neuroscience Symposium. New Orleans.
Thanks for this post! This is full of information and it sure looks like you know a lot about the brain! I have heard that some vitamins will help improve your memory. Is this true? What are the best memory enhancing supplements that I can get?
ReplyDelete