In recent years, the growing public concern over
deficiencies of schools has led a growing number of educations to embrace neuroscience.
Neuroscience is a discipline that integrates anatomy, physiology, psychology,
psychiatry, pathology as they relate to function of the nervous system,
particularly the brain. In 1969, I was one of 500 charter members of the
society that formalized the discipline, the Society for Neuroscience. But only
in the last decade has there been much interest in the potential for
neuroscience to influence educational policy and practice.
In educational circles, this
interest has been expressed with such terms as "brain-based learning,"
"educational neuroscience," and "neuro-education." The
latter term is used for my Linked-in group. Whatever it ends up being called, a
new discipline is growing. So far, both good and bad effects are manifest.
Numerous critics have pointed out
that neuroscience has not had much impact on education, and worse yet has
spawned a series of harmful myths. The myths arise frequently from well-meaning
people who have misunderstood and misapplied the findings of neuroscience. Sometimes,
the myths come from zealous neuroscientists who make false claims and promises.
More often, the myths come from educators who lack scientific training.
A recent review has identified some
of the more flagrant myths. Some beliefs are downright foolish and have no
research-based evidence. For example, there is the false (and untestable) claim
that we only use 10% of our brain. I have no idea where this absurdity arose,
but surely it was not from a professional neuroscientist. But one very
prominent scientist, whom I do not wish to embarrass, made an outrageous claim
that 95% of everything humans do is programmed and that basically we have no
free will to improve ourselves or do anything else. I challenge this idea that
free will is an illusion in my new book, now in the page-proof stage. The
existence of free will is essential for an ability to take personal
responsibility and be held accountable for one's beliefs, thoughts, and
actions. Belief that free will is an illusion is demotivating, leading children
to think they are irredeemable victims of their biology and environment. Neuroscience
research clearly establishes that the brain is readily changed by one's choices
of experience, thought, and behavior. Freedom to make beneficial choices is
empowering.
Another myth, not likely
attributable to a scientist, is the claim that the brain shrinks if a child
drinks less than six glasses of water each day and that this will cause
children to underperform in school.
Such myths have led some critics to
charge that neuroscience hype is destructive of sound educational practice. In
some cases, that charge is justified. For example, some neuroscientists and
educators have believed that children have differing learning styles and that
teachers need to adjust teaching to accommodate visual, auditory, and kinesthetic
learners. But controlled laboratory studies fail to confirm such biological
differences among children. Yet belief in individualized learning styles is
still believed by over 93-97% of teachers in five surveyed countries (U.K.,
China, Netherlands, Turkey, Greece). In those same countries, 71-91% of
teachers believe, without evidence, that individual differences among learners are
explained by differences in right-brain/left-brain dominance.
Many educators oppose IQ tests, presumably
because the score differentials discriminatively
label capability. This has given rise to the notion of "multiple
intelligences," which is an untestable hypothesis because of the
uncontrolled variables involved in defining the types and number of
intelligences. There is also a common belief that IQ is fixed and unchangeable,
yet evidence to the contrary is abundant.
Another mistaken belief is that the
early ages of 0-3 years old are a critical period wherein most brain
development occurs. There is no supporting evidence, and a vast amount of
evidence shows that the brain is in continual development throughout life, even
possible in old age. Brain development and learning capabilities develop at
different rates and times in different people.
Nonetheless, a belief in the
special importance of pre-K seems to be growing, even though there are
indications that some kinds of instruction, such as reading, achieve better success
when they are delayed. In Finland, noted for its excellence in education,
teaching policy aims at delaying didactic education; "pre-school"
education begins at age 6. Their kindergarten day lasts only four hours and is
filled mostly with play time and social activities. Teaching of reading may not
begin until age 7. Yet, in the U.S., Common Core standards require rigorous
language training in kindergarten. There is no evidence that children who are
taught to read in kindergarten have any long-term benefit from the early
exposure. Likewise, the latest evidence shows that "Head-start"
programs have no lasting impact, yet there is great public support for creating
pre-Head Start programs.
Two common learning disabilities, ADHD and
dyslexia, have been widely studied, both by educators and neuroscientists.
Unfortunately, confusion abounds. In the case of ADHD, there is belief that it
has to be treated with drugs and that it cannot be reduced by teaching or
behavioral therapy. Myths have surrounded dyslexia, ranging from ideas that it
does not exist to treatments based on the false notion that it was caused by a visual
perception deficit. The real cause seems to be a problem with phonological
coding.
As with any new scholarly
discipline, a degree of misunderstanding and hype should be expected with
educational neuroscience. The proper perspective is to be wary of false
prophets and snake oil, but open to the possibility that new knowledge in
neuroscience has genuine potential for enriching education practice and
outcomes. Research on the biology of memory, reviewed in one of my books, has clear beneficial potential for education that has not been exploited. Better and more informed interdisciplinary collaboration is needed if
we wish neuroscience to enrich rather than mislead education.
Sources:
1. Howard-Jones, Paul. A. (2014). Neuroscience and
education: myths and messages. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 15, 817-824.
2. History of SFN, 1969-1995. https://www.sfn.org/about/history-of-sfn/the-creation-of-neuroscience/establishing-the-society-for-neuroscience
3. Neuro-education: promoting cognitive development. https://www.linkedin.com/groups/4883556
4. Walker, Tim. (2015). The joyful, illiterate
kindergartners of Finland. http://www.theatlantic.com/education/archive/2015/10/the-joyful-illiterate-kindergartners-of-finland/408325/
5. Klemm, W. R. (2016). Making a Scientific Case for Free
Will. New York: Elsevier. In press.
6. Klemm, W. R. (2012). Memory Power 101. New York: Skyhorse.