Keep your "nose to the
grindstone" is the advice we often tell young people is an essential
ingredient of learning difficult tasks. A joke captures the matter with the old
bromide for success, "Keep your eye on the ball, your ear to the ground,
your nose to the grindstone, your shoulder to the wheel: Now try to work in
that position."
Over the years of teaching, I
have seen many highly conscientious students work like demons in their study
yet don't seem to learn as much as they should for all the effort they put in.
Typically, it is because they don't study smart.
In an earlier post, I described a
learning strategy wherein a student should spend short (say 15-20 minutes) of
intense study followed immediately by a comparable rest period of
"brain-dead" activity where they don't engage with intense stimuli or
a new learning task. The idea is that during brain down-time the memory of
just-learned material is more likely to be consolidated into long-term memory
because there are no mental distractions to erase the temporary working memory
while it is in the process of consolidation.
Now, new research suggests that
too much nose-to-the-grindstone can impair learning. Margaret Schlichting, a
graduate student researcher, and Alison Preston, an associate professor of
psychology and neuroscience at the University of Texas tested the effect of
mental rest with a learning task of remembering two sets of a series of
associated photo pairs. Between the two
task sets, the participants rested and were allowed to think about whatever
they wanted. Not surprisingly, those who used the rest time to reflect on what
they had just learned were able to remember more upon re-test. Obviously, in
this case, the brain is not really resting, as it is processing (that is,
rehearsing) the new learning. But the brain is resting in the sense that new
mental challenges are not encountered.
The university press release
quotes the authors as saying, "We've shown for the first time that how the
brain processes information during rest can improve future learning. We think
replaying memories during rest makes those earlier memories stronger, not just
impacting the original content, but impacting the memories to come." Despite
the fact that this concept has been anointed as a new discovery in a
prestigious science journal, the principle has been well-known for decades. I
have explained this phenomenon in my memory books as the decades-old term of "interference
theory of memory,"
What has not been well understood
among teachers is the need to alter teaching practices to accommodate this
principle. A typical class period involves teachers presenting a back-to-back
succession of highly diverse learning objects and concepts. Each new topic interferes
with memory formation of the prior topics. An additional interference occurs
when a class period is disrupted by blaring announcements from the principal's
office, designed to be loud to command attention (which has the effect of
diverting attention away from the learning material). The typical classroom has
a plethora of other distractions, such as windows for looking outside and
multiple objects like animals, pictures, posters, banners, and ceiling mobiles
designed to decorate and enliven the room. The room itself is a major
distraction.
Then, to compound the problem,
the class bell rings, and students rush out into the hall for their next class,
socializing furiously in the limited time they have to get to the next class
(on a different subject, by a different teacher, in a differently decorated
classroom). You can be sure, little reflection occurs on the academic material
they had just encountered.
The format of a typical school
day is so well-entrenched that I doubt it can be changed. But there is no
excuse for blaring loudspeaker announcements during the middle of a class
period. Classrooms do not have to be decorated. A given class period does not
have to be an information dump on overwhelmed students. Short periods of
instruction need to be followed by short, low-key, periods of questioning, discussion,
reflection, and application of what has just been taught. Content that doesn't
get "covered" in class can be assigned as homework—or even exempted
from being a learning requirement. It is better to learn a few things well than
many things poorly. Indeed, this is the refreshing philosophy behind the new
national science standards known as "Next Generation Science
Standards."
Give our kids a rest: the right
kind of mental rest.
Sources:
http://www.nextgenscience.org/
http://scicasts.com/neuroscience/2065-cognitive-science/8539-study-suggests-mental-rest-and-reflection-boost learning.
Schlicthing, M. L., and Preston, A. R. (2014). Memory reactivation during rest supports upcoming learning of related content. Proc. Nat. Acad. Science. Published ahead of print, Oct. 20.
Dr.
Klemm's latest book, available at most retail outlets, is "Mental Biology.
The New Science of How the Brain and Mind Relate" (Prometheus). See
reviews at http://thankyoubrain.com